Predicting the effects of stereotypes and biases

Earlier this week, PNAS published our paper presenting a computational model that predicts the effects of stereotypes and biases with an accuracy and precision that I would not have thought was possible even 2 years ago. I had some media requests that asked me to elaborate on the study for a more general audience. I figure it is worthwhile to post them here in addition for all to see:

What was the significance of this research? What did we learn?
Our study showed how to quantitatively predict how our stereotypes about other groups affect our treatment of these groups, and addressed two important outstanding questions when it comes to stereotypes and biases.

Stereotyping
First is the question of how to quantify and predict effects of stereotypes, which deal with intangible thoughts and beliefs, on tangible behavior and outcomes, measured for example in dollars and cents.

Second is the question of comparing across the advantages and disadvantages conferred by different stereotypes. For example, do stereotypes about one being "female" affect how one is treated more than say "Hispanic"? If so, how much more or less? This is particularly challenging as there are nearly infinite ways that people can stereotype different social groups.

Our results show that it is possible to do both. The basic idea is that stereotypes change the weight we place on the outcome of others. Some stereotypes increase how much we care about someone, some stereotypes decrease it. Moreover, all stereotypes can be organized along two dimensions: warmth and competence. For example, in the US, comparing stereotypes about an Irish person and a Japanese person. The Irish person is thought of as warmer but the Japanese person is thought of as more competent.

How did you arrive at these conclusions?
We developed a computational model that took in people's ratings about others' perceived warmth and competence, as well as the economic incentives that people are faced with. We then tested how people behaved toward members of other groups in laboratory experiments involving over 1,200 participants. Specifically, the degree to which certain groups were perceived as warm or competent conferred specific amount of benefit/penalty.

We then further validated this model could predict behavior in a previously published labor market field experiment involving thirteen thousand resumes. Specifically, using people's perceptions of warmth and competence, we were able to predict the likelihood that someone would be called back when applying for a job.

What Neuroscience Can Inform Us about Brand Equity II

Since most people these days don’t have the time to actually read papers, here is what I think are the highlights from our paper that might surprise people in terms of what neuroscience can inform us about brand equity.

While researching for the paper, something that struck me was the lack of agreement on even basic concepts of “brand equity”. Below is a (likely complete) list of dimensions of “customer-based” brand equity proposed in the literature. (Contrasted with firm-based brand equity which is typically calculated from financial data).

Brand_equity
What’s really striking is how they are so similar and different at the same time. I’m not trying to pull a post-modernism trick. For example, consider something like “brand awareness”, which flits in and out of the various frameworks. It clearly has some relationship with brand associations, since one can’t be aware of something that has no associations (or can it?). But what is that relationship and how is it distinct from brand associations? Trust me, it doesn’t get any better than that.

A basic contribution of neuroscience, then, is to simply provide a more rigorous framework to conceptualize these marketing typologies. No psychologist studying “knowledge”, “associations”, and “awareness” would be ignorant of the fundamental contribution cognitives neuroscience has made to these topics. I don’t think it’s crazy to suggest that the same should apply to consumer researchers. As for what I tentatively propose as a more biologically-plausible framework? That, as they say, requires you to read the paper.

More generally, I’ve found that a useful way to think about what neuroscience can contribute to marketing and consumer research is to ask, “What do consumer researchers think they are studying/measuring?” This might seem either obvious or unnecessarily antagonistic, depending on your perspective. But it is quite different from the way the questions as they are typically posed currently, which is more akin to “What can [enter your technique] tell me about [enter what marketer/consumer researcher is interested in]?” At the risk of making sweeping generalizations (which of course I will now be making), a real issue this runs into is that what marketers are interested in are often surprisingly ill-defined. I don’t think this is a knock on marketers because the real-world is complicated. Plenty of health researchers study “resilience” but nobody really knows how to define it. The real problem is in refusing to entertain the possibility that one can provide a more rigorous scientific foundation of these “fuzzy” concepts.

What Neuroscience Can Inform Us about Brand Equity

In the next couple of entries, I’m going to talk about my new paper on what neuroscience can inform us about brand equity. It will be in Moran Cerf and Manuel Garcia’s Consumer Neuroscience book, to be published by MIT Press. Here is the abstract:

Hsu, Ming. Customer-based brand equity: Insights from consumer neuroscience. in Moran Cerf and Manuel Garcia (eds.) Consumer Neuroscience. MIT Press, Forthcoming.

This review describes how insights from cognitive and behavioral neurosciences are helping to organize and interpret the relationship between consumers and brands. Two components of brand equity—consumer brand knowledge and consumer responses—are discussed. First, it is argued that consumer brand knowledge consists of multiple forms of memories that are encoded in the brain, including well-established forms of semantic (attributes and associations) and episodic (experiences and feelings) memory. Next, it is argued that there exist distinct forms of consumer responses that correspond to distinct behavioral systems—a goal-directed system that captures valence of attitudes and preference for a brand, and a habit system that captures previously learned values but no longer reflect ongoing preferences. Finally, a neuroscientifically-grounded conceptualization is proposed with the aim of ultimately improving measurement of brand equity and its effects on financial returns.